Spatial updating in virtual environments: What are vestibular cues good for?

Bernhard E. Riecke, Markus von der Heyde, Heinrich Bulthoff

Research output: Contribution to journalArticle

3 Citations (Scopus)

Abstract

When we turn ourselves, our sensory inputs somehow turn the "world inside our head" accordingly so as to stay in alignment with the outside world. This "spatial updating" occurs automatically, without conscious effort, and is normally " obligatory" (i.e., cognitively impenetrable and hard to suppress). We pursued two main questions here: 1) Which cues are sufficient to initiate obligatory spatial updating? 2) Under what circumstances do vestibular cues become important? Stimuli: A photo-realistic virtual replica of the Tübingen market place was presented via a curved projection screen (84×63° FOV). For vestibular stimulation, subjects were seated on a Stewart motion platform. Task: Subjects were rotated consecutively to random orientations and asked to point "as accurately and quickly as possible" to 4 out of 22 previously-learned targets. Targets were announced consecutively via headphones and chosen to be outside of the current FOV. Photo-realistic visual stimuli from a well-known environment including an abundance of salient landmarks allowed accurate spatial updating (mean absolute pointing error, pointing variability, and response time were 16.5°, 17.0°, and 1.19s, respectively). Moreover, those stimuli triggered spatial updating even when participants were asked to ignore turn cues and "point as if not having turned", (32.9°, 27.5°, 1.67s, respectively). Removing vestibular turn cues did not alter performance significantly. This result conflicts with the prevailing opinion that vestibular cues are required for proper updating of ego-turns. We did find that spatial updating benefitted from vestibular cues when visual turn information was degraded to a mere optic flow pattern. Under all optic flow conditions, however, spatial updating was impaired and no longer obligatory. We conclude that " good" visual landmarks can initiate obligatory spatial updating and overcome the visuo-vestibular cue conflict.

Original languageEnglish
JournalJournal of Vision
Volume2
Issue number7
DOIs
Publication statusPublished - 2002 Dec 1

Fingerprint

Cues
Optic Flow
Ego
Reaction Time
Head

ASJC Scopus subject areas

  • Ophthalmology

Cite this

Spatial updating in virtual environments : What are vestibular cues good for? / Riecke, Bernhard E.; von der Heyde, Markus; Bulthoff, Heinrich.

In: Journal of Vision, Vol. 2, No. 7, 01.12.2002.

Research output: Contribution to journalArticle

@article{6312a284af064e8897b0805ea2d1bca6,
title = "Spatial updating in virtual environments: What are vestibular cues good for?",
abstract = "When we turn ourselves, our sensory inputs somehow turn the {"}world inside our head{"} accordingly so as to stay in alignment with the outside world. This {"}spatial updating{"} occurs automatically, without conscious effort, and is normally {"} obligatory{"} (i.e., cognitively impenetrable and hard to suppress). We pursued two main questions here: 1) Which cues are sufficient to initiate obligatory spatial updating? 2) Under what circumstances do vestibular cues become important? Stimuli: A photo-realistic virtual replica of the T{\"u}bingen market place was presented via a curved projection screen (84×63° FOV). For vestibular stimulation, subjects were seated on a Stewart motion platform. Task: Subjects were rotated consecutively to random orientations and asked to point {"}as accurately and quickly as possible{"} to 4 out of 22 previously-learned targets. Targets were announced consecutively via headphones and chosen to be outside of the current FOV. Photo-realistic visual stimuli from a well-known environment including an abundance of salient landmarks allowed accurate spatial updating (mean absolute pointing error, pointing variability, and response time were 16.5°, 17.0°, and 1.19s, respectively). Moreover, those stimuli triggered spatial updating even when participants were asked to ignore turn cues and {"}point as if not having turned{"}, (32.9°, 27.5°, 1.67s, respectively). Removing vestibular turn cues did not alter performance significantly. This result conflicts with the prevailing opinion that vestibular cues are required for proper updating of ego-turns. We did find that spatial updating benefitted from vestibular cues when visual turn information was degraded to a mere optic flow pattern. Under all optic flow conditions, however, spatial updating was impaired and no longer obligatory. We conclude that {"} good{"} visual landmarks can initiate obligatory spatial updating and overcome the visuo-vestibular cue conflict.",
author = "Riecke, {Bernhard E.} and {von der Heyde}, Markus and Heinrich Bulthoff",
year = "2002",
month = "12",
day = "1",
doi = "10.1167/2.7.421",
language = "English",
volume = "2",
journal = "Journal of Vision",
issn = "1534-7362",
publisher = "Association for Research in Vision and Ophthalmology Inc.",
number = "7",

}

TY - JOUR

T1 - Spatial updating in virtual environments

T2 - What are vestibular cues good for?

AU - Riecke, Bernhard E.

AU - von der Heyde, Markus

AU - Bulthoff, Heinrich

PY - 2002/12/1

Y1 - 2002/12/1

N2 - When we turn ourselves, our sensory inputs somehow turn the "world inside our head" accordingly so as to stay in alignment with the outside world. This "spatial updating" occurs automatically, without conscious effort, and is normally " obligatory" (i.e., cognitively impenetrable and hard to suppress). We pursued two main questions here: 1) Which cues are sufficient to initiate obligatory spatial updating? 2) Under what circumstances do vestibular cues become important? Stimuli: A photo-realistic virtual replica of the Tübingen market place was presented via a curved projection screen (84×63° FOV). For vestibular stimulation, subjects were seated on a Stewart motion platform. Task: Subjects were rotated consecutively to random orientations and asked to point "as accurately and quickly as possible" to 4 out of 22 previously-learned targets. Targets were announced consecutively via headphones and chosen to be outside of the current FOV. Photo-realistic visual stimuli from a well-known environment including an abundance of salient landmarks allowed accurate spatial updating (mean absolute pointing error, pointing variability, and response time were 16.5°, 17.0°, and 1.19s, respectively). Moreover, those stimuli triggered spatial updating even when participants were asked to ignore turn cues and "point as if not having turned", (32.9°, 27.5°, 1.67s, respectively). Removing vestibular turn cues did not alter performance significantly. This result conflicts with the prevailing opinion that vestibular cues are required for proper updating of ego-turns. We did find that spatial updating benefitted from vestibular cues when visual turn information was degraded to a mere optic flow pattern. Under all optic flow conditions, however, spatial updating was impaired and no longer obligatory. We conclude that " good" visual landmarks can initiate obligatory spatial updating and overcome the visuo-vestibular cue conflict.

AB - When we turn ourselves, our sensory inputs somehow turn the "world inside our head" accordingly so as to stay in alignment with the outside world. This "spatial updating" occurs automatically, without conscious effort, and is normally " obligatory" (i.e., cognitively impenetrable and hard to suppress). We pursued two main questions here: 1) Which cues are sufficient to initiate obligatory spatial updating? 2) Under what circumstances do vestibular cues become important? Stimuli: A photo-realistic virtual replica of the Tübingen market place was presented via a curved projection screen (84×63° FOV). For vestibular stimulation, subjects were seated on a Stewart motion platform. Task: Subjects were rotated consecutively to random orientations and asked to point "as accurately and quickly as possible" to 4 out of 22 previously-learned targets. Targets were announced consecutively via headphones and chosen to be outside of the current FOV. Photo-realistic visual stimuli from a well-known environment including an abundance of salient landmarks allowed accurate spatial updating (mean absolute pointing error, pointing variability, and response time were 16.5°, 17.0°, and 1.19s, respectively). Moreover, those stimuli triggered spatial updating even when participants were asked to ignore turn cues and "point as if not having turned", (32.9°, 27.5°, 1.67s, respectively). Removing vestibular turn cues did not alter performance significantly. This result conflicts with the prevailing opinion that vestibular cues are required for proper updating of ego-turns. We did find that spatial updating benefitted from vestibular cues when visual turn information was degraded to a mere optic flow pattern. Under all optic flow conditions, however, spatial updating was impaired and no longer obligatory. We conclude that " good" visual landmarks can initiate obligatory spatial updating and overcome the visuo-vestibular cue conflict.

UR - http://www.scopus.com/inward/record.url?scp=4043147926&partnerID=8YFLogxK

UR - http://www.scopus.com/inward/citedby.url?scp=4043147926&partnerID=8YFLogxK

U2 - 10.1167/2.7.421

DO - 10.1167/2.7.421

M3 - Article

AN - SCOPUS:4043147926

VL - 2

JO - Journal of Vision

JF - Journal of Vision

SN - 1534-7362

IS - 7

ER -